Language acquisition is the process by which humans acquire the capacity to perceive and comprehend language, as well as to produce and use words and sentences to communicate. Language acquisition is one of the quintessential human traits, because nonhumans do not communicate by using language.[1] Language acquisition usually refers to first-language acquisition, which studies infants' acquisition of their native language. This is distinguished from second-language acquisition, which deals with the acquisition (in both children and adults) of additional languages.
Language might be vocalized as speech or manual as in sign.
The human language capacity is represented in the brain. Even though
the human language capacity is finite, one can say and understand an
infinite number of sentences, which is based on a syntactic principle
called Recursion.
Evidence suggests that every individual has three recursive mechanisms
that allow sentences to go indeterminately. These three mechanisms are: relativization, complementation and coordination.[2]
The capacity to acquire and use language is a key aspect that
distinguishes humans from other beings. Although it is difficult to pin
down what aspects of language are uniquely human, there are a few design
features that can be found in all known forms of human language, but
that are missing from forms of animal communication.
For example, many animals are able to communicate with each other by
signaling to the things around them, but this kind of communication
lacks the arbitrariness of human vernaculars (in that there is nothing
about the sound of the word "dog" that would hint at its meaning). Other
forms of animal communication may utilize arbitrary sounds, but are
unable to combine those sounds in different ways to create completely
novel messages that can then be automatically understood by another. Hockett
called this design feature of human language "productivity". It is
crucial to the understanding of human language acquisition that we are
not limited to a finite set of words, but, rather, must be able to
understand and utilize a complex system that allows for an infinite
number of possible messages. So, while many forms of animal
communication exist, they differ from human languages, in that they have
a limited range of non-syntactically structured vocabulary tokens that
lack cross cultural variation between groups.[3]
A major question in understanding language acquisition is how these capacities are picked up by infants from the linguistic input. Input in the linguistic context
is defined as "All words, contexts, and other forms of language to
which a learner is exposed, relative to acquired proficiency in first or
second languages". Nativists find it difficult to believe, considering the hugely complex nature of human languages, and the relatively limited cognitive abilities
of an infant, that infants are able to acquire most aspects of language
without being explicitly taught. At a very young age, children can
already distinguish between different sounds but cannot produce them
yet. However, during infancy, children do begin to babble. It is
important to note that deaf babies babble in the same order when hearing
sounds as non-deaf babies do. This is evident of the fact that babbling
is not caused by babies simply imitating certain sounds, but is
actually a natural part of the process of language development. However,
it has been found that deaf babies do often babble less than non-deaf
babies and they begin to babble later on in infancy (begin babbling at
11 months as compared to 6 months) when compared to non-deaf babies. [4] Children, within a few years of birth, understand the grammatical rules of their native language without being explicitly taught, as one learns grammar in school.[5]
A range of theories of language acquisition have been proposed in order
to explain this apparent problem. These theories, championed by the
likes of Noam Chomsky and others, include innatism and Psychological nativism,
in which a child is born prepared in some manner with these capacities,
as opposed to other theories in which language is simply learned as
other cognitive skills, including such mundane motor skills as learning
to ride a bike. The conflict between the theories assuming humans are
born with syntactic knowledge and those that claim all such knowledge is
the product of learning from one's environment is often referred to as
the "Nature vs. Nurture"
debate. Some think that there are some qualities of language
acquisition that the human brain is automatically wired for (a "nature"
component) and some that are shaped by the particular language
environment in which a person is raised (a "nurture" component). Others,
especially evolutionary biologists, strongly object to assuming
syntactic knowledge is genetically encoded and provided by automatic
wiring of the brain.
No comments:
Post a Comment